7.7 Voluntary
Participation and Informed Consent
An
individual is entitled to decline to participate in any research
project or to terminate participation at any time. Participation
in an experiment, survey, or focus group is always voluntary and
any form of coercion is unacceptable. Researchers who are in a
position of authority over subjects (as in the situation where
the researcher hands the university students questionnaires)
should be especially sensitive to implied coercion: even
though the researcher might tell the class that failure to
participate will not affect their grades, many students may not
believe this. In such a situation, it would be advisable to keep
the questionnaires anonymous and to have the person in authority
be absent from the room while the survey is administered.
Voluntary
participation
is a less pressing ethical issue in mail and telephone surveys,
since respondents are free to hang up the phone or to throw away
the questionnaire. Nonetheless, a researcher should not attempt
to induce subjects to participate by misrepresenting the
organization sponsoring the research or by exaggerating its
purpose or importance. For example, phone interviewers should
not be instructed to identify themselves as representatives of
the "Department of Information" to mislead people into thinking
the survey is government-sponsored. Likewise, mail
questionnaires should not be constructed to mimic census forms,
tax returns, social security questionnaires, or other official
government forms.
Closely
related to voluntary participation is the notion
of informed consent. For people to volunteer for a
research project, they need to know enough about the project to
make an intelligent choice. Researchers have the responsibility
to inform potential subjects or respondents of all features of
the project that can reasonably be expected to influence
participation. Respondents should understand that an interview
may take as long as 45 minutes, or that a second interview is
required, or that upon completing a mail questionnaire, they may
be singled out for a telephone interview.
In
an experiment,
informed consent means that potential subjects must be warned of
any possible discomfort or unpleasantness that might be
involved. Subjects should be told if they are to receive or
administer electric shocks, be subjected to unpleasant audio or
visual stimuli, or undergo any procedure that may cause concern.
Any unusual measurement techniques that may be used also must be
described. Researchers have an obligation to answer candidly and
truthfully, as far as possible, all the participant's questions
about the research.
Experiments
that involve deception (see the following subsection) cause
special problems with regard to obtaining informed consent. If
deception is absolutely necessary to conduct an experiment, is
the experimenter obligated to inform subjects that they may be
deceived during the upcoming experiment? Will such a disclosure
affect participation in the experiment? Will it also affect the
experimental results? Should one compromise by telling all
potential subjects that deception to be involved for some
participants but not for others?
A
second problem
is deciding exactly how much information about a research
project must be disclosed in seeking to achieve informed
consent. Is it enough to explain that the experiment involves
rating commercials, or is it necessary to add that the
experiment is designed to test whether subjects with high IQs
prefer different commercials from those with low IQs?
Obi-\\ovs\f, msorrie situations the researcher cannot reveal
everything about the project for fear of contaminating the
results. For example, if the goal of the research is to examine
the influence of peer pressure on commercial evaluations,
alerting the subjects to this facet of the investigation might
change their behavior in the experiment.
Problems
might occur
in research examining the impact of mass media in non-literate
communities, for example, if the research subjects did not
comprehend what they were told regarding the proposed
investigation. Even in literate societies, many people fail to
understand the implications for confidentiality of the storage
of survey data on computer disks or tape. Moreover, an
investigator might not have realized in advance that some
subjects would find part of an experiment or survey emotionally
disturbing. Since it is impossible for informed consent to apply
to all situations, the American Psychological Association has
suggested that researchers have a responsibility to continue
their attention to subjects' welfare after the completion of
data collection.
Research
findings
provide some indication of what research participants should be
told. Subjects always want a general description of the
experiment and what was expected of them; they want to know
whether danger was involved, how long the experiment would last,
and the experiment's purpose. As far as informed consent and
survey participation are concerned. There is a wide variation
among researchers about what to tell respondents in the survey
introduction. Almost all introductions identified the research
organization and the interviewer by name and described the
research topic. Less frequently mentioned in introductions were
the sponsor of the research and guarantees of confidentiality or
anonymity. Few survey introductions mentioned the length of the
survey or that participation was voluntary.
Finally,
one must consider the form of the consent to be obtained.
Written consent is a requirement in certain government-sponsored
research programs and may also be required by many university
research review committees, as discussed next in connection with
guidelines promulgated by the federal government. In several
generally recognized situations, however, signed forms are
regarded as impractical. These include telephone surveys, mail
surveys, personal interviews, and cases in which the signed form
itself might represent an occasion for breach of
confidentiality. For example, a respondent who has been promised
anonymity as an inducement to participate in a face-to-face
interview might be suspicious if asked to sign a consent form
after the interview. In these circumstances, the fact that the
respondent agreed to participate is taken as implied consent. |
|
7.8 Concealment and Deception
Concealment and deception techniques are encountered most
frequently in experimental research. Concealment is the
withholding of certain information from the subjects; deception
is deliberately providing false information. Both practices
raise ethical problems. The difficulty in obtaining consent has
already been mentioned. A second problem derives from the
general feeling that it is wrong for experimenters to lie or
otherwise to deceive subjects.
Many
critics
argue that deception transforms a subject from a human being
into a manipulated object and is therefore demeaning to the
participant. Moreover, once subjects have been deceived, they
are likely to expect to be deceived again in other research
projects. At least two research studies seem to suggest that
this concern is valid. Studies have found that high
incidence of suspicion among subjects of high school age after
having been deceived.
On
the other hand,
some researchers argue that certain studies could not be
conducted at all without the use of deception. They claim that
the harm done to those who are deceived is outweighed by the
benefits of the research to scientific knowledge. The same
arguments can be used both for and against concealment. In
general, however, concealment is a somewhat less worrisome
ethical problem, provided enough information is given to
subjects to allow informed consent and all the subjects'
questions are answered candidly.
Obviously,
deception is not a technique that should be used
indiscriminately. It is suggested that before the investigator
settles on deception as an experimental tactic, three questions
should be examined:

1.
How significant is the proposed study?
2.
Are alternative procedures available that would provide the same
information?
3.
How severe is the deception? (It is one thing to tell subjects
that the experimentally constructed message they are reading was
taken from the New York Times; it is another to report
that the test a subject has just completed was designed to
measure latent suicidal tendencies.)
Another set of criteria was put forth by Elms (1982), who
suggested five necessary and sufficient conditions under which
deception can be considered ethically justified in social
science research.

1.
When there is no other feasible way to obtain the desired
information
2.
When the likely benefits substantially outweigh the likely harms
3.
When subjects are given the option to withdraw at any time
without penalty
4.
When any physical or psychological harm to subjects is temporary
5.
When subjects are debriefed as to all substantial deception and
the research procedures are made available for public review
Researchers are offered good advice for the planning stages of
investigations.
When an experiment is concluded, especially one involving
concealment or deception, it is the responsibility of the
investigator to debrief subjects. Debriefing should be thorough
enough to remove any lasting effects that might have been
created by the experimental manipulation or by any other aspect
of the experiment. Subjects' questions should be answered and
the potential value of the experiment stressed. How common is
debriefing among mass media researchers? |
|
7.9 Protection of
Privacy
The
problem of protecting the privacy of participants usually occurs
more often in survey research than in laboratory studies.
Subjects have a right to know whether their privacy will be
maintained and who will have access to the information they
provide. There are two ways to guarantee privacy: by assuring
anonymity and by assuring confidentiality. A promise of
anonymity is a guarantee that a given respondent cannot possibly
be linked to any particular response. In many research projects
anonymity is an advantage, since it encourages respondents to be
honest and candid in their answers. Strictly speaking, personal
and telephone interviews cannot be anonymous because the
researcher can link a given questionnaire to a specific person,
household, or telephone number. In such instances, the
researcher should promise confidentiality; that is, the
respondents should be assured that even though as individuals
they can be identified, their names will never be publicly
associated with the information they provide. A researcher
should never use "anonymous" in a way that is or seems to be
synonymous with "confidential."
Additionally,
respondents should be told who will have access to the
information they provide. The researcher's responsibility for
assuring confidentiality does not end once the data have been
analyzed and the study concluded. Questionnaires that identify
persons by name should not be stored in public places, nor
should other investigators be given permission to examine
confidential data unless all identifying marks have been
obliterated. |
|
7.10 Ethics in Data
Analysis and Reporting
Researchers are also responsible for maintaining professional
standards in the analysis and reporting of their data. The
ethical guidelines in this area are less controversial and more
clear-cut. One cardinal rule is that researchers have a moral
and ethical obligation to refrain from tampering with data:
questionnaire responses and experimental observations may not be
fabricated, altered, or discarded. Similarly, researchers are
expected to maintain reasonable care in processing the data to
guard against needless errors that might affect the results.
Researchers
should never conceal information that might influence the
interpretation of their findings. For example, if two weeks
elapsed between the testing of the experimental group and the
testing of the control group, this delay should be reported so
that other researchers can discount the effects of history and
maturation on the results. Every research report should contain
a full and complete description of method, particularly of any
departure from standard procedures.
Since
science is a public activity,
researchers have an ethical obligation to share their findings
and methods with other researchers. All questionnaires,
experimental materials, measurement instruments, instructions to
subjects, and other relevant items should be made available to
those who wish to examine them.
Finally,
all investigators are under an ethical obligation
to draw conclusions from their data that are consistent with
those data. Interpretations should not be stretched or distorted
to fit a personal point of view or a favorite theory, or to gain
or maintain a client's favor. Nor should researchers attribute
greater significance or credibility to their data than they
justify For example, when analyzing correlation coefficients
obtained from a large sample, it is possible to achieve
statistical significance with an r of only, for example, 10. It
would be perfectly acceptable to report a statistically
significant result in this case, but the investigator should
also mention that the predictive utility of the correlation was
not large, and specifically, that it explained only 1% of the
total variation. In short, researchers should report results
with candor and honesty. |
|
7.11 Finding Support
for Research
Research
costs money.
Finding a source for research funds is a problem that confronts
both quantitative and qualitative researchers in all fields of
social science.
A
researcher in need of funding
should contact these organizations for details about the types
of studies they support and the amount of funds available, as
well as instructions for preparing research proposals.
University
or college researchers
should determine whether the institution has a program of
research grants for individual faculty members. Many colleges
award such grants, often on a competitive basis, for social
research. Typically these grants are modest in size — usually
under $5,000 — but they are among the easiest to apply for and
to administer. In many cases grants are available for student
research as well.
Finally,
most colleges and universities have an Office of Contracts and
Grants (or some similar title) that can be of great help to
researchers. In addition to aiding the researcher with the
bureaucratic requirements necessary for a grant application,
this office can offer valuable assistance in other areas. For
example, this office might offer computerized searches for
sponsoring agencies, information about current grants, budget
advice, preparation of abstracts, and even word-processing
services. Researchers in the academic setting should take
advantage of this resource.
|
|
|
|
|